Real World Applications of Random Variables and Bayes' Rule in CSE 312 Spring Lecture 9

random variables bayes rule applications n.w
1 / 39
Embed
Share

Explore the practical implications of random variables and Bayes' Rule in real-world scenarios, including understanding infinite processes, calculating probabilities, and defining random variables. Learn methods to find the probability of certain outcomes, such as at least 3 heads in a coin-flipping scenario. Dive into the concept of random variables and their significance in summarizing numerical information from outcomes.

  • Random Variables
  • Bayes Rule
  • Probability
  • CSE 312
  • Real World Applications

Uploaded on | 0 Views


Download Presentation

Please find below an Image/Link to download the presentation.

The content on the website is provided AS IS for your information and personal use only. It may not be sold, licensed, or shared on other websites without obtaining consent from the author. If you encounter any issues during the download, it is possible that the publisher has removed the file from their server.

You are allowed to download the files provided on this website for personal or commercial use, subject to the condition that they are used lawfully. All files are the property of their respective owners.

The content on the website is provided AS IS for your information and personal use only. It may not be sold, licensed, or shared on other websites without obtaining consent from the author.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Random Variables Bayes Rule Applications CSE 312 Spring 24 Lecture 9

  2. Today A convenient representation: random variables Bayes Rule in the real world!

  3. Implicitly defining We ve often skipped an explicit definition of . Often | | is infinite, so we really couldn t write it out (even in principle). How would that happen? Flip a fair coin (independently each time) until you see your first tails. what is the probability that you see at least 3 heads?

  4. An infinite process. is infinite. A sequential process is also going to be infinite But the tree is self-similar To know what the next step looks like, you only need to look back a finite number of steps. From every node, the children look identical (H with probability , continue pattern; T to a leaf with probability ) ? =1 ? =1 2 2 H ? = 1/2 ??|? =1 ??|? =1 2 2 H ?? = 1/4 ???|?? =1 ???|?? =1 2 2 H ??? = 1/8

  5. Finding (at least 3 heads) Method 1: infinite sum. includes ??? for every ?. Every such outcome has probability 1/2?+1 What outcomes are in our event? 1 1 1/2=1 24 1/2?+1= ?=3 Infinite geometric series, where common ratio is between 1 and 1 has closed form first term 1 ratio 8

  6. Finding (at least 3 heads) Method 2: Calculate the complement (at most 2 heads) = 1 2+1 4+1 8 1 2+1 4+1 =1 (at least 3 heads)= 1 8 8

  7. Random Variables

  8. Random Variable What s a random variable? Formally Random Variable ?: is a random variable ?(?) is the summary of the outcome ? Informally: A random variable is a way to summarize (numerical) information from your outcome. summarize the important

  9. The sum of two dice EVENTS We could define ?2= sum is 2 ?3= sum is 3 ?12= sum is 12 RANDOM VARIABLE ?: ? is the sum of the two dice. And ask which event occurs ?

  10. More random variables From one sample space, you can define many random variables. Roll a fair red die and a fair blue die Let ? be the value of the red die minus the blue die ? 4,2 = 2 Let ? be the sum of the values of the dice ? 4,2 = 6 Let ? be the maximum of the values ? 4,2 = 4

  11. Notational Notes We will always use capital letters for random variables. It s common to use lower-case letters for the values they could take on. Formally Formally random variables are functions, so you d think we d write ? ?,?,? = 2 But we nearly never do. We just write ? = 2

  12. Support (?) The support (aka the range ) is the set of values ? can actually take. We called this the image in 311. ? (difference of red and blue) has support { 5, 4, 3, ,4,5} ? (sum) has support {2,3, ,12} What is the support of ? (max of the two dice)

  13. Probability Mass Function Often we re interested in the event {?:?(?) = ?} Which is the event that ? = ?. We ll write (? = ?) to describe the probability of that event So ? = 2 = 1 36, ? = 7 =1 6 The function that tells you (? = ?)is the probability mass function We ll often write p?? for the pmf. probability mass function

  14. Partition A random variable partitions . D2=1 D2=2 D2=3 D2=4 D2=5 D2=6 (1,1) (1,2) (1,3) (2,1) (2,2) (2,3) (3,1) (3,2) (3,3) (4,1) (4,2) (4,3) (5,1) (5,2) (5,3) (6,1) (6,2) (6,3) D1=1 D1=2 D1=3 D1=4 D1=5 D1=6 (1,4) (2,4) (3,4) (4,4) (5,4) (6,4) (1,5) (2,5) (3,5) (4,5) (5,5) (6,5) (1.6) (2,6) (3,6) (4,6) (5,6) (6,6) Let ? be the number of twos in rolling a (fair) red and blue die. ??0 = 25/36 ??1 = 10/36 ??2 = 1/36

  15. Try It Yourself There are 20 balls, numbered 1,2, ,20 in an urn. You ll draw out a size-three subset. (i.e. without replacement) = {size three subsets of 1, ,20}, () is uniform measure. Let ? be the largest value among the three balls. If outcome is {4,2,10} then ? = 10. Write down the pmf of ? Fill out the poll everywhere so Robbie knows how long to explain Go to pollev.com/robbie

  16. Try It Yourself There are 20 balls, numbered 1,2, ,20 in an urn. You ll draw out a size-three subset. (i.e. without replacement) Let ? be the largest value among the three balls. 3 if ? , 3 ? 20 ? 1 2 0 /20 ??? = otherwise Good check: if you sum up ??(?) do you get 1? Good check: is ??? 0 for all ?? Is it defined for all ??

  17. Bayes in the real world

  18. Application 1: Medical Tests Helping Doctors and Patients Make Sense of Health Statistics A researcher posed the following scenario to a group of 160 doctors: Assume you conduct a disease screening using a standard test in a certain region. You know the following information about the people in this region: The probability that a person has the disease is 1% (prevalence) If a person has the disease, the probability that she tests positive is 90% (sensitivity) If a person does not have the disease, the probability that she nevertheless tests positive is 9% (false-positive rate) A person tests positive. She wants to know from you whether that means that she has the disease for sure, or what the chances are. What is the best answer? A. The probability that she has the disease is about 81%. B. Out of 10 people with a positive test, about 9 have the disease. C. Out of 10 people with a positive test, about 1 have the disease. D. The probability that she has the disease is about 1%

  19. Lets do the calculation! Let ?be the patient has the disease , ? be the test was positive. ? ? = ? ? ? / (?) = .99 .09+ .01 .9 0.092 .9 .01 Calculation tip: for Bayes Rule, you should see one of the terms on the bottom exactly match your numerator (if you re using the LTP to calculate the probability on the bottom)

  20. Pause for vocabulary Physicians have words for just about everything Let ? be has the disease; ? be test is positive (?)is prevalence (?|?)is sensitivity A sensitive test is one which picks up on the disease when it s there (high sensitivity -> few false negatives) ? ?is specificity A specific test is one that is positive specifically because of the disease, and for no other reason (high specificity -> few false positives)

  21. How did the doctors do C (about 1 in 10) was the correct answer. Of the doctors surveyed, less than got it right (so worse than random guessing). After the researcher taught them his calculation trick, more than 80% got it right.

  22. One Weird Trick! Calculation Trick: imagine you have a large population (not one person) and ask how many there are of false/true positives/negatives.

  23. What about the real world? When you re older and have to do more routine medical tests, don t get concerned (yet) when they ask to run another test.* It s usually fine.* *This is not medical advice, Robbie is not a physician.

  24. Careful Surveys

  25. Application 2: An Imbalanced Survey In 2014, a paper was published Do non-citizens vote in U.S. elections? This is a real paper (peer-reviewed). It claims that 1. In a survey, about 4% (of a few hundred) of non-U.S.-citizens surveyed said they voted in the 2008 federal election (which isn t allowed). 2. Those non-citizen voters voted heavily (estimate 80+%) for democrats. 3. It is likely though by no means certain that John McCain would have won North Carolina were it not for the votes for Obama cast by non-citizens

  26. Application 2: What is this survey? The Cooperative Congressional Election Study was run in 2008 and 2010. It interviews about 20,000 people about how/whether they voted in federal elections. Two strange observations: 1. The noncitizens are a very small portion of those surveyed. Feels a little strange. 2. Those people maybe accidentally admitted to a crime?

  27. Application 2: Another Red Flag A response paper (by different authors) The perils of cherry picking low frequency events in large sample surveys

  28. An Explanation Suppose 0.1% of people check the wrong check-box on any individual question (independently) Suppose you really interviewed 20,000 people, of whom 300 are really non-citizens (none of whom voted), and the rest are citizens, of whom 70% voted. What is the probability someone appears to have voted ??? ?? ??? ? (??? ?) (??? ??) .001 .7 300 20000)+.001 (19700 ??? ? ??? ?? = = 20000) .999 ( 4.38%

  29. Conclusion The authors of the original paper did know about response error and they have an appendix that argues the population of non-citizen voters isn t distributed exactly like you d expect. But with it being such a small number of people, this isn t surprising. And even they admit response bias played more of a role than they initially thought. Though they still think they found some evidence of non-citizens voting (but not enough to flip North Carolina anymore).

  30. Takeaways When talking about rare events (rare diseases, rare prize-winning- golden-tickets), think carefully about whether a test is really as informative as you think. Do the explicit calculation Intuition is easier if thinking about a large population of repeated tests, not just one. Be careful of small subparts of large datasets People from a large majority group (accidentally) clicking the wrong demographic information can drown out signal of a very small group.

  31. Optional: Bayes Factor A way to estimate Bayes estimate Bayes calculations quickly

  32. Bayes Factor Another Intuition Trick: from 3Blue1Brown from 3Blue1Brown When you test positive, you (approximately Bayes Factor (aka likelihood ratio) approximately) multiply the prior by the sensitivity false positive rate=1 ??? ???

  33. Bayes Factor Does it work? Let s try it Find prior Sensitivity ???

  34. Wonka Bars Willy Wonka has placed golden tickets on 0.1% of his Wonka Bars. You want to get a golden ticket. You could buy a 1000-or-so of the bars until you find one, but that s expensive you ve got a better idea! You have a test a very precise scale you ve bought. If the bar you weigh does does have a golden ticket, the scale will alert you 99.9% of the time. If the bar you weigh does not have a golden ticket, the scale will (falsely) alert you only 1% of the time. If you pick up a bar and it alerts, what is the probability you have a golden ticket?

  35. Wonka Bars Bayes Factor 99.9 1 Prior: .1% Product: 9.99, so about 10% About what Bayes Rule gets!

  36. Application 1: Medical Tests Helping Doctors and Patients Make Sense of Health Statistics A researcher posed the following scenario to a group of 160 doctors: Assume you conduct a disease screening using a standard test in a certain region. You know the following information about the people in this region: The probability that a person has the disease is 1% (prevalence) If a person has the disease, the probability that she tests positive is 90% (sensitivity) If a person does not have the disease, the probability that she nevertheless tests positive is 9% (false-positive rate) A person tests positive. She wants to know from you whether that means that she has the disease for sure, or what the chances are. What is the best answer? A. The probability that she has the disease is about 81%. B. Out of 10 people with a positive test, about 9 have the disease. C. Out of 10 people with a positive test, about 1 have the disease. D. The probability that she has the disease is about 1%

  37. Bayes Factor What about with the doctors? 1% 90% 9%= 10% Again about right!

  38. Caution Multiplying by the Bayes Factor is an approximation It gives you the exact numerator for Bayes, but the denominator is the number of false positives if the prevalence (/prior) were 0 approximation When the prior is close to 0, this is a fine approximation! But plug in a prior of 15% on the last slide, and we get 150% chance.

  39. What about negative tests? For negative tests, the Bayes Factor is ??? specificity Specificity is (1 false negative rate)

Related


More Related Content